Eliciting Risk and Time Preferences
Steffen Andersen
Dept. of Economics and Centre for Economic and Business Research, Copenhagen Business School, Copenhagen, Denmark; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorGlenn W. Harrison
Dept. of Economics, College of Business Administration, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816, U.S.A. and Durham Business School, Durham University, Durham, U.K.; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorMorten I. Lau
Dept. of Economics and Finance, Durham Business School, Durham University, Durham, U.K.; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorE. Elisabet Rutström
Dept. of Economics, College of Business Administration, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816, U.S.A.; [email protected]
Harrison and Rutström thank the U.S. National Science Foundation for research support under grants NSF/IIS 9817518, NSF/HSD 0527675, and NSF/SES 0616746. Harrison and Lau thank the Danish Social Science Research Council for research support under project 24-02-0124. We are grateful to Peter Wakker, Nathaniel Wilcox, six anonymous referees, and David Levine for helpful comments. Supporting data and instructions are stored at the ExLab Digital Archive located at http://exlab.bus.ucf.edu.
Search for more papers by this authorSteffen Andersen
Dept. of Economics and Centre for Economic and Business Research, Copenhagen Business School, Copenhagen, Denmark; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorGlenn W. Harrison
Dept. of Economics, College of Business Administration, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816, U.S.A. and Durham Business School, Durham University, Durham, U.K.; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorMorten I. Lau
Dept. of Economics and Finance, Durham Business School, Durham University, Durham, U.K.; [email protected] ,
Search for more papers by this authorE. Elisabet Rutström
Dept. of Economics, College of Business Administration, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816, U.S.A.; [email protected]
Harrison and Rutström thank the U.S. National Science Foundation for research support under grants NSF/IIS 9817518, NSF/HSD 0527675, and NSF/SES 0616746. Harrison and Lau thank the Danish Social Science Research Council for research support under project 24-02-0124. We are grateful to Peter Wakker, Nathaniel Wilcox, six anonymous referees, and David Levine for helpful comments. Supporting data and instructions are stored at the ExLab Digital Archive located at http://exlab.bus.ucf.edu.
Search for more papers by this authorAbstract
We design experiments to jointly elicit risk and time preferences for the adult Danish population. Since subjects are generally risk averse, we find that joint elicitation provides estimates of discount rates that are significantly lower than those found in previous studies and more in line with what would be considered as a priori reasonable rates. The statistical specification relies on a theoretical framework that involves a latent trade-off between long-run optimization and short-run temptation. Estimation of this specification is undertaken using structural, maximum likelihood methods. Our main results based on exponential discounting are robust to alternative specifications such as hyperbolic discounting. These results have direct implications for attempts to elicit time preferences, as well as debates over the appropriate domain of the utility function when characterizing risk aversion and time consistency.
References
-
Abdulkadri, A. O., and
M. R. Langemeier (2000): “Using Farm Consumption Data to Estimate the Intertemporal Elasticity of Substitution and Relative Risk Aversion Coefficients,”
Agricultural Finance Review, 60, 61–70.
10.1108/00214690080001110 Google Scholar
- Ahlbrecht, M., and M. Weber (1997): “An Empirical Study on Intertemporal Decision Making Under Risk,” Management Science, 43, 813–826.
- Ainslie, G. (1992): Picoeconomics. New York : Cambridge University Press.
-
Anderhub, V.,
W. Güth,
U. Gneezy, and
D. Sonsino (2001): “On the Interaction of Risk and Time Preference: An Experimental Study,”
German Economic Review, 2, 239–253.
10.1111/1468-0475.00036 Google Scholar
- Andersen, S., G. W. Harrison, M. I. Lau, and E. E. Rutström (2006): “Elicitation Using Multiple Price Lists,” Experimental Economics, 9, 383–405.
- Andersen, S., G. W. Harrison, M. I. Lau, and E. E. Rutström (2008a): “ Supplement to ‘Eliciting Risk and Time Preferences’,” Econometrica Supplemental Material, Vol. 76, http://www.econometricsociety.org/ecta/Supmat/6966_instructions to experimental subjects.pdf; http://www.econometricsociety.org/ecta/Supmat/6966_data.zip.
- Andersen, S., G. W. Harrison, M. I. Lau, and E. E. Rutström (2008b): “Lost in State Space: Are Preferences Stable? International Economic Review, 49, forthcoming.
- Ballinger, T. P., and N. T. Wilcox (1997): “Decisions, Error and Heterogeneity,” Economic Journal, 107, 1090–1105.
- Barrett, L. F., M. M. Tugade, and R. W. Engle (2004): “Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Dual-Process Theories of the Mind,” Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553–573.
- Benhabib, J., and A. Bisin (2005): “Modeling Internal Commitment Mechanisms and Self-Control: A Neuroeconomics Approach to Consumption-Saving Decisions,” Games and Economic Behavior, 52, 460–492.
-
Cameron, A. C., and
P. K. Trivedi (2005): Microeconometrics: Methods and Applications.
New York
: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511811241 Google Scholar
- Cappellari, L., and S. P. Jenkins (2006): “Calculation of Multivariate Normal Probabilities by Simulation, With Applications to Maximum Simulated Likelihood Estimation,” Stata Journal, 6, 156–189.
- Chapman, G. (1996): “Temporal Discounting and Utility for Health and Money,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 771–791.
- Chavas, J.-P. (2004): Risk Analysis in Theory and Practice. San Diego , CA : Elsevier Academic Press.
-
Cohen, J. D. (2005): “The Vulcanization of the Human Brain: A Neural Perspective on Interactions Between Cognition and Emotion,”
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 3–24.
10.1257/089533005775196750 Google Scholar
-
Coller, M., and
M. B. Williams (1999): “Eliciting Individual Discount Rates,”
Experimental Economics, 2, 107–127.
10.1023/A:1009986005690 Google Scholar
- Eckel, C., C. Johnson, and C. Montmarquette (2005): “ Savings Decisions of the Working Poor: Short- and Long-Term Horizons,” in Field Experiments in Economics, Research in Experimental Economics, Vol. 10, ed. by J. Carpenter, G. W. Harrison, and J. A. List. Greenwich , CT : JAI Press, 219–260.
- El-Gamal, M. A., and D. M. Grether (1995): “Are People Bayesian? Uncovering Behavioral Strategies,” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90, 1137–1145.
- Everitt, B. S. (1996): “An Introduction to Finite Mixture Distributions,” Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 5, 107–127.
- Frederick, S., G. Loewenstein, and T. O'Donoghue (2002): “Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review,” Journal of Economic Literature, XL, 351–401.
- Fudenberg, D., and D. K. Levine (2006): “A Dual-Self Model of Impulse Control,” American Economic Review, 96, 1449–1476.
- Fudenberg, D., and D. K. Levine (2007): “ Self Control, Risk Aversion and the Allais Paradox,” Unpublished Manuscript, Department of Economics, Washington University at St. Louis .
-
Gollier, C. (2001): The Economics of Risk and Time.
Cambridge
,
MA
: MIT Press.
10.7551/mitpress/2622.001.0001 Google Scholar
- Gouriéroux, C., and A. Monfort (1996): Statistics and Econometric Models. Cambridge , U.K. : Cambridge University Press.
- Hamermesh, D. S. and G. A. Pfann (eds.) (2005): The Economics of Time Use, Contributions to Economic Analysis, Vol. 271. Amsterdam : Elsevier.
- Harless, D. W., and C. F. Camerer (1994): “The Predictive Utility of Generalized Utility Theories,” Econometrica, 62, 1251–1289.
- Harrison, G. W., and J. A. List (2004): “Field Experiments,” Journal of Economic Literature, 42, 1013–1059.
- Harrison, G. W., and E. E. Rutström (2005): “Expected Utility Theory and Prospect Theory: One Wedding and a Decent Funeral,” Working Paper 05-18, Department of Economics, College of Business Administration, University of Central Florida ; Experimental Economics, forthcoming.
-
Harrison, G. W., and
E. E. Rutström (2008): “ Risk Aversion in the Laboratory,”
in
Risk Aversion in Experiments, Research in Experimental Economics, Vol. 12, ed. by
J. C. Cox and
G. W. Harrison.
Bingley
,
U.K.
: Emerald, forthcoming.
10.1016/S0193-2306(08)00003-3 Google Scholar
- Harrison, G. W., R. M. Harstad, and E. E. Rutström (2004): “Experimental Methods and Elicitation of Values,” Experimental Economics, 7, 123–140.
- Harrison, G. W., M. I. Lau, and E. E. Rutström (2007): “Estimating Risk Attitudes in Denmark: A Field Experiment,” Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 109, 341–368.
-
Harrison, G. W.,
M. I. Lau,
E. E. Rutström, and
M. B. Sullivan (2005): “ Eliciting Risk and Time Preferences Using Field Experiments: Some Methodological Issues,”
in
Field Experiments in Economics, Research in Experimental Economics, Vol. 10, ed. by
J. Carpenter,
G. W. Harrison, and
J. A. List.
Greenwich
,
CT
: JAI Press, 125–218.
10.1016/S0193-2306(2005)10 Google Scholar
- Harrison, G. W., M. I. Lau, and M. B. Williams (2002): “Estimating Individual Discount Rates for Denmark: A Field Experiment,” American Economic Review, 92, 1606–1617.
- Herrnstein, R. J. (1981): “ Self-Control as Response Strength,” in Quantification of Steady-State Operant Behavior, ed. by C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi, and C. F. Lowe. Amsterdam : North-Holland, 36–52.
- Hey, J. D. (1995): “Experimental Investigations of Errors in Decision Making Under Risk,” European Economic Review, 39, 633–640.
- Hey, J. D. (2002): “Experimental Economics and the Theory of Decision Making Under Uncertainty,” Geneva Papers on Risk and Insurance Theory, 27, 5–21.
- Hey, J. D., and C. Orme (1994): “Investigating Generalizations of Expected Utility Theory Using Experimental Data,” Econometrica, 62, 1291–1326.
- Holt, C. A., and S. K. Laury (2002): “Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects,” American Economic Review, 92, 1644–1655.
- Holt, C. A., and S. K. Laury (2005): “Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects: New Data Without Order Effects,” American Economic Review, 95, 902–912.
- Issler, J. V., and N. S. Piqueira (2000): “Estimating Relative Risk Aversion, the Discount Rate, and the Intertemporal Elasticity of Substitution in Consumption for Brazil Using Three Types of Utility Functions,” Brazilian Review of Econometrics, 20, 201–239.
- Kahneman, D., J. L. Knetsch, and R. H. Thaler (1990): “Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem,” Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325–1348.
- Keller, L. R., and E. Strazzera (2002): “Examining Predictive Accuracy Among Discounting Models,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 24, 143–160.
- Kirby, K. N., and N. N. Maraković (1996): “Delay-Discounting Probabilistic Rewards: Rates Decrease as Amounts Increase,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 100–104.
- Kirby, K. N., and M. Santiesteban (2003): “Concave Utility, Transactions Costs, and Risk in Measuring Discounting of Delayed Rewards,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 66–79.
- Kreps, D. M., and E. L. Porteus (1978): “Temporal Resolution of Uncertainty and Dynamic Choice Theory,” Econometrica, 46, 185–200.
- Liang, K.-Y., and S. L. Zeger (1986): “Longitudinal Data Analysis Using Generalized Linear Models,” Biometrika, 73, 13–22.
- Loomes, G., and R. Sugden (1995): “Incorporating a Stochastic Element Into Decision Theories,” European Economic Review, 39, 641–648.
- Loomes, G., P. G. Moffatt, and R. Sugden (2002): “A Microeconometric Test of Alternative Stochastic Theories of Risky Choice,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 24, 103–130.
- Mazur, J. E. (1987): “ An Adjustment Procedure for Studying Delayed Reinforcement,” in The Effect of Delay and Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value, ed. by M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, and H. Rachlin. Hillsdale , NJ : Erlbaum, 55–76.
-
McLachlan, G., and
D. Peel (2000): Finite Mixture Models.
New York
: Wiley.
10.1002/0471721182 Google Scholar
- Mitchell, R. C., and R. T. Carson (1989): Using Surveys to Value Public Goods: The Contingent Valuation Method. Baltimore : Johns Hopkins Press.
- Papke, L. E., and J. M. Wooldridge (1996): “Econometric Methods for Fractional Response Variables With an Application to 401(K) Plan Participation Rates,” Journal of Applied Econometrics, 11, 619–632.
-
Pearson, K. (1894): “Contribution to the Mathematical Theory of Evolution,”
Philosophical Transactions A, 185, 71–110.
10.1098/rsta.1894.0003 Google Scholar
- Prelec, D. (2004): “Decreasing Impatience: A Criterion for Non-Stationary Time Preference and “Hyperbolic” Discounting,” Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 106, 511–532.
- Rabin, M. (2000): “Risk Aversion and Expected Utility Theory: A Calibration Theorem,” Econometrica, 68, 1281–1292.
- Rogers, W. H. (1993): “Regression Standard Errors in Clustered Samples,” Stata Technical Bulletin, 13, 19–23.
- Shapiro, J. M. (2005): “Is There a Daily Discount Rate? Evidence From the Food Stamp Nutrition Cycle,” Journal of Public Economics, 89, 303–325.
- Tanaka, T., C. F. Camerer, and Q. Nguyen (2007): “ Risk and Time Preferences: Experimental and Household Survey Data From Vietnam,” Working Paper, California Institute of Technology .
-
Train, K. E. (2003): Discrete Choice Methods With Simulation.
New York
: Cambridge University Press.
10.1017/CBO9780511753930 Google Scholar
- Van Praag, B. M. S., and A. S. Booij (2003): “ Risk Aversion and the Subjective Time Preference: A Joint Approach,” Working Paper 923, CESifo Institute for Economic Studies , Munich .
- Wakker, P. P. (2006): “ Explaining the Characteristics of the Power (CRRA) Utility Family,” Unpublished Manuscript, Econometric Institute, Erasmus University , Rotterdam .
- Williams, R. L. (2000): “A Note on Robust Variance Estimation for Cluster-Correlated Data,” Biometrics, 56, 645–646.
- Wooldridge, J. (2003): “Cluster-Sample Methods in Applied Econometrics,” American Economic Review, 93, 133–138.